Open Access
Issue |
SHS Web Conf.
Volume 143, 2022
Colloque international « Enseigner et apprendre l’orthographe à l’heure du numérique » (ONEA)
|
|
---|---|---|
Article Number | 01003 | |
Number of page(s) | 8 | |
Section | Activité de l’élève et processus d’apprentissage | |
DOI | https://doi.org/10.1051/shsconf/202214301003 | |
Published online | 04 August 2022 |
- Bekebrede, J., Leij, A. et Share, D. (2009). Dutch dyslexic adolescents : Phonological-core variable-orthographic differences. Reading and Writing, 22, 133–165. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-007-9105-7 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Borchardt, G. (2013). L’influence des connaissances graphotactiques sur l’acquisition de l’orthographe lexicale : Étude chez l’enfant d’école élémentaire et chez l’adulte [Université Paris Descartes]. https://jeannicod.ccsd.cnrs.fr/STAR/tel-00801214 [Google Scholar]
- Bosse, M.-L. et Valdois, S. (2009). Influence of the visual attention span on child reading performance : A crosssectional study. Journal of Research in Reading, 32(2), 230–253. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9817.2008.01387.x [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Cadario, R., Butori, R. et Parguel, B. (2017). Chapitre 8. Tests des effets médiateurs. Dans Méthode expérimentale. Analyses de modération et médiation (p. 181-210). Bruxelles ; De Boeck. [Google Scholar]
- Caravolas, M., Bruck, M. et Genesee, F. (2003). Similarities and differences between English- and Frenchspeaking poor spellers. In Dyslexia in different languages : Cross-linguistic comparisons (p. 157-180). Whurr Publishers. [Google Scholar]
- Caravolas, M., Hulme, C. et Snowling, M. J. (2001). The Foundations of Spelling Ability: Evidence from a 3-Year Longitudinal Study. Journal of Memory and Language, 45(4), 751–774. https://doi.org/10.1006/jmla.2000.2785 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Carrillo, M. S. et Alegría, J. (2014). The development of children’s sensitivity to bigram frequencies when spelling in Spanish, a transparent writing system. Reading and Writing, 27(3), 571–590. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-013-9459-y [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Castles, A. et Nation, K. (2006). How does orthographic learning happen? Dans S. Andrews (ed.), From inkmarks to ideas: Current issues in lexical processing (p. 151-179). Psychology Press. [Google Scholar]
- Coltheart, M. (1978). Lexical access in simple reading tasks. Dans G. Underwood (ed.), Strategies of information processing (p. 151-216). Academic Press. [Google Scholar]
- Commissaire, E. et Besse, A.-S. (2019). Investigating lexical and sub-lexical orthographic processing skills in French 3rd and 5th graders. Journal of Research in Reading, 42(2), 268–287. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9817.12268 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Conrad, N. J. et Deacon, S. H. (2016). Children’s Orthographic Knowledge and Their Word Reading Skill: Testing Bidirectional Relations. Scientific Studies of Reading, 20(4), 339–347. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888438.2016.1183128 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Cunningham, A. E., Perry, K. E. et Stanovich, K. E. (2001). Converging evidence for the concept of orthographic processing. Reading and Writing, 14(5), 549–568. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1011100226798 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Cunningham, A. E. et Stanovich, K. E. (1990). Assessing print exposure and orthographic processing skill in children: A quick measure of reading experience. Journal of Educational Psychology, 82(4), 733–740. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.82.4.733 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Cunningham, A. E. et Stanovich, K. E. (1991). Tracking the unique effects of print exposure in children: Associations with vocabulary, general knowledge, and spelling. Journal of Educational Psychology, 83(2), 264–274. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.83.2.264 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Deacon. (2012). Sounds, letters and meanings: The independent influences of phonological, morphological and orthographic skills on early word reading accuracy. Journal of Research in Reading, 35(4), 456–475. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9817.2010.01496.x [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Deacon, S. H., Benere, J. et Castles, A. (2012). Chicken or egg? Untangling the relationship between orthographic processing skill and reading accuracy. Cognition, 122(1), 110–117. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2011.09.003 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Deacon, S. H., Leblanc, D. et Sabourin, C. (2011). When cues collide: Children’s sensitivity to letter- and meaning-patterns in spelling words in English. Journal of Child Language, 38(4), 809–827. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000910000322 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Dunn, L. M., Dunn, L. M. et Thériault-Whalen, C. M. (1993). Échelle de vocabulaire en images Peabody : EVIP. PSYCAN. [Google Scholar]
- Ehri, L. C. (2005). Learning to Read Words: Theory, Findings, and Issues. Scientific Studies of Reading, 9(2), 167–188. https://doi.org/10.1207/s1532799xssr0902_4 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Folco, C. D., Guez, A., Peyre, H. et Ramus, F. (2020). Epidemiology of developmental dyslexia: A comparison of DSM-5 and ICD-11 criteria. MedRxiv, 2020.12.18.20248189. https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.12.18.20248189 [Google Scholar]
- Frey, A. et Bosse, M.-L. (2018). Perceptual span, visual span, and visual attention span: Three potential ways to quantify limits on visual processing during reading. Visual Cognition, 26(6), 412–429. https://doi.org/10.1080/13506285.2018.1472163 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Frith, U. (1980). Unexpected spelling problems. Dans Cognitive processes in spelling (p. 495-515). Academic Press. [Google Scholar]
- Georgiou, G. K., Parrila, R. et Papadopoulos, T. C. (2008). Predictors of word decoding and reading fluency across languages varying in orthographic consistency. Journal of Educational Psychology, 100(3), 566–580. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.100.3.566 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Geva, E. et Wang, M. (2001). The development of basic reading skills in children: A cross-language perspective. Annual Review of Applied Linguistics, 21, 182–204. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0267190501000113 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Hagiliassis, N., Pratt, C. et Johnston, M. (2006). Orthographic and Phonological Processes in Reading. Reading and Writing, 19(3), 235–263. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-005-4123-9 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Hanley, J. R., Hastie, K. et Kay, J. (1992). Developmental Surface Dyslexia and Dysgraphia: An Orthographic Processing Impairment. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A, 44(2), 285–319. https://doi.org/10.1080/02724989243000046 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Jacquier-Roux, M., Pouget, G., Valdois, S. et Zorman, M. (2010). BALE : batterie analytique du langage écrit. Groupe Cogni-Sciences, Laboratoire de Psychologie et NeuroCognition. [Google Scholar]
- Kamhi, A. G. et Hinton, L. N. (2000). Explaining individual differences in spelling ability. Topics in Language Disorders, 20(3), 37–49. https://doi.org/10.1097/00011363-200020030-00006 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- McCloskey, M. et Rapp, B. (2017). Developmental dysgraphia: An overview and framework for research. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 34(3-4), 65-82. https://doi.org/10.1080/02643294.2017.1369016 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Morton, J. et Patterson, K. E. (1980). A new attempt at an interpretation or an attempt at a new interpretation. Dans M. Coltheart, K. E. Patterson et J. C. Marshall (eds.), Deep dyslexia (p. 91-118). Routledge et Kegan Paul. [Google Scholar]
- Ocal, T. et Ehri, L. (2017). Spelling Ability in College Students Predicted by Decoding, Print Exposure, and Vocabulary. Journal of College Reading and Learning, 47(1), 58–74. https://doi.org/10.1080/10790195.2016.1219242 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Olson, R., Forsberg, H., Wise, B. et Rack, J. (1994). Measurement of word recognition, orthographic, and phonological skills. Dans G. R. Lyon (ed.), Frames of reference for the assessment of learning disabilities: New views on measurement issues (p. 243-277). Paul H. Brookes Publishing Co. [Google Scholar]
- Pacton, S., Borchardt, G., Treiman, R., Lété, B. et Fayol, M. (2014). Learning to spell from reading: General knowledge about spelling patterns influences memory for specific words. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 67(5), 1019–1036. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2013.846392 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Pritchard, S. C., Coltheart, M., Marinus, E. et Castles, A. (2018). A Computational Model of the Self-Teaching Hypothesis Based on the Dual-Route Cascaded Model of Reading. Cognitive Science, 42(3), 722–770. https://doi.org/10.1111/cogs.12571 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Rakhlin, N., Cardoso-Martins, C. et Grigorenko, E. L. (2014). Phonemic Awareness Is a More Important Predictor of Orthographic Processing Than Rapid Serial Naming: Evidence From Russian. Scientific Studies of Reading, 18(6), 395–414. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888438.2014.918981 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Raven, J. C., Court, J. H. et Raven, J. E. (1998). Raven’s coloured progressive matrices. Harcourt Assessment. [Google Scholar]
- Roman, A. A., Kirby, J. R., Parrila, R. K., Wade-Woolley, L. et Deacon, S. H. (2009). Toward a comprehensive view of the skills involved in word reading in Grades 4, 6, and 8. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 102(1), 96–113. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2008.01.004 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Rothe, J., Cornell, S., Ise, E. et Schulte-Körne, G. (2015). A comparison of orthographic processing in children with and without reading and spelling disorder in a regular orthography. Reading and Writing, 28(9), 1307–1332. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-015-9572-1 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Share, D. L. (1995). Phonological recoding and self-teaching: Sine qua non of reading acquisition. Cognition, 55(2), 151–218. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(94)00645-2 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Stainthorp, R., Powell, D. et Stuart, M. (2013). The relationship between rapid naming and word spelling in English. Journal of Research in Reading, 36(4), 371–388. https://doi.org/10.1111/jrir.12002 [Google Scholar]
- Stanovich, K. E. et West, R. F. (1989). Exposure to Print and Orthographic Processing. Reading Research Quarterly, 24(4), 402–433. https://doi.org/10.2307/747605 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Taylor, K. K. et Kidder, E. B. (1988). The Development of Spelling Skills: From First Grade through Eighth Grade. Written Communication, 5(2), 222–244. https://doi.org/10.1177/0741088388005002006 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Treiman, R. (1997). Spelling in normal children and dyslexics. Dans B. A. Blachman (ed.), Foundations of reading acquisition and dyslexia: Implications for early interventions (p. 191-218). Mahwah, NJ: LEA. [Google Scholar]
- Treiman, R., Kessler, B. et Bick, S. (2002). Context sensitivity in the spelling of English vowels. Journal of Memory and Language, 47(3), 448–468. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0749-596X(02)00010-4 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
- Valente, M. J., Pelham, W. E., Smyth, H. et MacKinnon, D. P. (2017). Confounding in Statistical Mediation Analysis: What It Is and How to Address It. Journal of counseling psychology, 64(6), 659–671. https://doi.org/10.1037/cou0000242 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
Current usage metrics show cumulative count of Article Views (full-text article views including HTML views, PDF and ePub downloads, according to the available data) and Abstracts Views on Vision4Press platform.
Data correspond to usage on the plateform after 2015. The current usage metrics is available 48-96 hours after online publication and is updated daily on week days.
Initial download of the metrics may take a while.